Edited by Susan Fiske, Princeton University, Jamaica, VT; received April 26, 2025; accepted August 3, 2025
August 26, 2025
122 (35) e2510318122
Edited by Susan Fiske, Princeton University, Jamaica, VT; received April 26, 2025; accepted August 3, 2025
August 26, 2025
122 (35) e2510318122
Significance
What signals a life well lived? We addressed this question by examining millions of obituaries over time, across cultural events, and exploring how legacies were modified by demographics of the deceased. The most prevalent personal value in obituaries was tradition (e.g., focusing on religion). Cultural events, like the COVID-19 pandemic, linked to changes in legacy reflections as well. For example, care for close others (benevolence) decreased throughout the pandemic and never recovered, even 4 y after its start. Finally, legacies of women showed minimal fluctuations with increased age; legacies of men showed more dynamic and systematic age-related changes over time. Obituaries tell an important psychological and cultural story about how societies remember others and what constitutes a meaningful life.
Abstract
How societies remember the dead can reveal what people value in life. We analyzed 38 million obituaries from the United States to examine how personal values are encoded in individual and collective legacies. Using Schwartz’s theory of basic human values, we found that tradition and benevolence dominated legacy reflections, while values like power and stimulation appeared less frequently. Major cultural events—the terrorist attacks of September 11th, the 2008 financial crisis, and the COVID-19 pandemic—were systematically linked to changes in legacy reflections about personal values, with security declining after 9/11, achievement declining after the financial crisis, and benevolence declining for years after COVID-19 began and, to date, not yet returning to baseline. Gender and age of the deceased were also linked to differences in legacy: Men were remembered more for achievement, power, and conformity, while women were remembered more for benevolence and hedonism. Older people were remembered more for tradition and conformity than younger people. These patterns shifted dynamically across the lifespan, with obituaries for men showing more age-related variation than legacies for women. Our findings reveal how obituaries serve as psychological and cultural time capsules, preserving not just individual legacies, but also indicating what US society values collectively regarding a life well lived.
Get full access to this article
Purchase, subscribe or recommend this article to your librarian.
Data, Materials, and Software Availability
Based on the terms of the data acquisition agreement, underlying data for this study cannot be made publicly available. However, those who are interested in forming a collaboration with the present authors to tackle additional empirical questions should contact the corresponding author (D.M.M.) to discuss this process. If a project commences, the authors may provide limited access to a sample of the data for analytic purposes. Direct access to the data (for a fee) from Legacy.com can be arranged by contacting personnel at [email protected].
Acknowledgments
Author contributions
D.M.M., S.S., and L.Y. designed research; D.M.M. performed research; D.M.M. analyzed data; and D.M.M., T.M., S.S., K.F.L., and L.Y. wrote the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interest.
Supporting Information
References
1
B. Waggoner, J. M. Bering, J. Halberstadt, The desire to be remembered: A review and analysis of legacy motivations and behaviors. New Ideas Psychol. 69, 101005 (2023).
2
L. Braudy, The Frenzy of Renown: Fame and Its History (Vintage Books, 1986).
3
M. Fox, L. P. Tost, K. A. Wade-Benzoni, The legacy motive: A catalyst for sustainable decision making in organizations. Bus. Ethics Q. 20, 153–185 (2010).
4
K. A. Wade-Benzoni, L. P. Tost, The egoism and altruism of intergenerational behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 13, 165–193 (2009).
5
E. G. Hunter, G. D. Rowles, Leaving a legacy: Toward a typology. J. Aging Stud. 19, 327–347 (2005).
6
M. Fox, K. A. Wade-Benzoni, J. G. Covin, Legacy: The meaning of lasting impact for family, business, and beyond. J. Fam. Bus. Strat. 15, 100633 (2024).
7
K. A. Wade-Benzoni, Legacy motivations & the psychology of intergenerational decisions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 26, 19–22 (2019).
8
L. Zaval, E. M. Markowitz, E. U. Weber, How will I be remembered? Conserving the environment for the sake of one’s legacy. Psychol. Sci. 26, 231–236 (2015).
9
S. Solomon, J. Greenberg, T. A. Pyszczynski, The Worm at the Core: On the Role of Death in Life (Random House, 2015).
10
T. Pyszczynski, J. Greenberg, S. Solomon, A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychol. Rev. 106, 835–845 (1999).
11
E. Pattee et al., Covid-19 makes us think about our mortality. Our brains aren’t designed for that. Washington Post, 7 October 2020.
12
A. Frias, P. C. Watkins, A. C. Webber, J. J. Froh, Death and gratitude: Death reflection enhances gratitude. J. Posit. Psychol. 6, 154–162 (2011).
13
S. H. Schwartz, “Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed. (Academic Press, 1992), pp. 1–65.
14
M. Rokeach, The Nature of Human Values (Free Press, 1973).
15
V. Ponizovskiy et al., Development and validation of the personal values dictionary: A theory-driven tool for investigating references to basic human values in text. Eur. J. Pers. 34, 885–902 (2020).
16
M. Alfano, A. Higgins, J. Levernier, Identifying virtues and values through obituary data-mining. J. Value Inquiry 52, 59–79 (2018).
17
J. Hume, Obituaries in American Culture (University Press of Mississippi, 2000).
18
N. Starck, Life After Death (Melbourne University Press, 2006).
19
20
S. Syropoulos, H. M. Watkins, G. P. Goodwin, E. M. Markowitz, Disentangling the contributions of impact-oriented versus reputation-focused legacy motives on intergenerational concern and action. J. Environ. Psychol. 90, 102092 (2023).
21
H. M. Bang, C. Zhou Koval, K. A. Wade-Benzoni, It’s the thought that counts over time: The interplay of intent, outcome, stewardship, and legacy motivations in intergenerational reciprocity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 73, 197–210 (2017).
22
R. L. Boyd, D. M. Markowitz, Verbal behavior and the future of social science. Am. Psychol. 80, 411–433 (2025).
23
R. L. Boyd, H. A. Schwartz, Natural language analysis and the psychology of verbal behavior: The past, present, and future states of the field. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 40, 21–41 (2021).
24
J. W. Pennebaker, The Secret Life of Pronouns: What Our Words Say About Us (Bloomsbury Press, 2011).
25
M. E. Ireland, M. Mehl, ”Natural language use as a marker of personality” in The Oxford Handbook of Language and Social Psychology, T. M. Holtgraves, Ed. (Oxford University Press, 2014), pp. 201–218.
26
M. Atari et al., The paucity of morality in everyday talk. Sci. Rep. 13, 5967 (2023).
27
M. Dehghani, R. L. Boyd, Handbook of Language Analysis in Psychology (Guilford Press, 2022).
28
D. M. Markowitz, Gender and ethnicity bias in medicine: A text analysis of 1.8 million critical care records. PNAS Nexus 1, pgac157 (2022).
29
A. Demzik et al., Linguistic differences in personal statements of urology residency applicants by self-reported race and ethnicity. Urology 162, 137–143 (2022).
30
R. Voigt et al., Language from police body camera footage shows racial disparities in officer respect. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 6521–6526 (2017).
31
D. M. Markowitz, A. Kittelman, E. Girvan, M. Santiago-Rosario, K. McIntosh, Taking note of our biases: How language patterns reveal bias underlying the use of office discipline referrals in exclusionary discipline. Educ. Res. 52, 525–534 (2023).
32
K. N. Jordan, J. Sterling, J. W. Pennebaker, R. L. Boyd, Examining long-term trends in politics and culture through language of political leaders and cultural institutions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 3476–3481 (2019).
33
A. Marks, T. Piggee, Obituary analysis and describing a life lived: The impact of race, gender, age, and economic status. Omega (Westport) 38, 37–57 (1999).
34
K. K. Maybury, Invisible lives: Women, men and obituaries. Omega (Westport) 32, 27–37 (1996).
35
M. E. W. Varnum, N. Baumard, M. Atari, K. Gray, Large Language Models based on historical text could offer informative tools for behavioral science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 121, e2407639121 (2024).
36
37
B. Fowler, Mapping the obituary: Notes towards a bourdieusian interpretation. Sociol. Rev. 52, 148–171 (2004).
38
S. Schwartz, An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Read. Psychol. Cult. 2, 1–20 (2012).
39
L. Sagiv, S. H. Schwartz, Personal values across cultures. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 73, 517–546 (2022).
40
C.-M. Vauclair, K. Hanke, R. Fischer, J. Fontaine, The structure of human values at the culture level: A meta-analytical replication of Schwartz’s value orientations using the Rokeach Value Survey. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 42, 186–205 (2011).
41
S. Schwartz, A theory of cultural value orientations: Explication and applications. Comp. Sociol. 5, 137–182 (2006).
42
M. A. Cohn, M. R. Mehl, J. W. Pennebaker, Linguistic markers of psychological change surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychol. Sci. 15, 687–693 (2004).
43
D. M. Markowitz, Psychological trauma and emotional upheaval as revealed in academic writing: The case of COVID-19. Cogn. Emot. 36, 9–22 (2022).
44
A. E. Abele, The dynamics of masculine-agentic and feminine-communal traits: Findings from a prospective study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 768–776 (2003).
45
A. E. Abele, B. Wojciszke, “Communal and agentic content in social cognition: A dual perspective model” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, J. M. Olson, M. P. Zanna, Eds. (Academic Press, 2014), pp. 195–255.
46
A. M. Koenig, A. H. Eagly, Evidence for the social role theory of stereotype content: Observations of groups’ roles shape stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 107, 371–392 (2014).
47
A. J. C. Cuddy, S. T. Fiske, P. Glick, “Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna Ed. (Academic Press, 2008), pp. 61–149.
48
M. Barreto, D. M. Doyle, Benevolent and hostile sexism in a shifting global context. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2, 98–111 (2023).
49
S. T. Fiske, “Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination” in The Handbook of Social Psychology, D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, G. Lindzey, Eds. (McGraw-Hill, ed. 4, 1998), vol. 1–2, pp. 357–411.
50
M. B. Brewer, “A dual process model of impression formation” in A Dual Process Model of Impression Formation, Advances in Social Cognition, T. K. Srull, R. S. Wyer, Eds. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., 1988), vol. 1, pp. 1–36.
51
R. Kastenbaum, S. Peyton, B. Kastenbaum, Sex discrimination after death. Omega (Westport) 7, 351–359 (1977).
52
M. C. Kearl, Death as a measure of life: A research note on the Kastenbaum–Spilka strategy of obituary analyses. Omega (Westport) 17, 65–78 (1987).
53
54
A. Deaton, The financial crisis and the well-being of Americans: 2011 OEP Hicks lecture. Oxf. Econ. Pap. 64, 1–26 (2012).
55
O. Giuntella, K. Hyde, S. Saccardo, S. Sadoff, Lifestyle and mental health disruptions during COVID-19. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118, e2016632118 (2021).
56
A. Case, A. Deaton, Rising morbidity and mortality in midlife among white non-Hispanic Americans in the 21st century. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 15078–15083 (2015).
57
The New York Times, nytimes/covid-19-data (2025). Deposited 10 June 2025.
58
C. K. Chung, J. W. Pennebaker, Revealing dimensions of thinking in open-ended self-descriptions: An automated meaning extraction method for natural language. J. Res. Pers. 42, 96–132 (2008).
59
D. M. Markowitz, The meaning extraction method: An approach to evaluate content patterns from large-scale language data. Front. Commun. 6, 13–13 (2021).
60
O. Burkeman, Four Thousand Weeks: Time Management for Mortals (Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2021).
61
J. J. V. Bavel et al., Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 460–471 (2020).
62
63
S. Groep, K. Zanolie, K. H. Green, S. W. Sweijen, E. A. Crone, A daily diary study on adolescents’ mood, empathy, and prosocial behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE 15, e0240349 (2020).
64
P. E. McKenna-Plumley, L. Graham-Wisener, E. Berry, J. M. Groarke, Connection, constraint, and coping: A qualitative study of experiences of loneliness during the COVID-19 lockdown in the UK. PLoS ONE 16, e0258344 (2021).
65
J. Greenberg, T. Pyszczynski, S. Solomon, “The causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A Terror Management Theory” in Public Self and Private Self, R. F. Baumeister, Ed. (Springer, 1986), pp. 189–212.
66
J. L. Goldenberg, S. K. McCoy, T. Pyszczynski, J. Greenberg, S. Solomon, The body as a source of self-esteem: The effect of mortality salience on identification with one’s body, interest in sex, and appearance monitoring. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 118–130 (2000).
67
C. Routledge et al., Adjusting to death: The effects of mortality salience and self-esteem on psychological well-being, growth motivation, and maladaptive behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 897–916 (2010).
68
H. Zhao, H. Zhang, Z. Wang, W. He, Does mortality salience make youths more materialistic? The role of future orientation and awe. Motiv. Emot. 47, 1007–1026 (2023).
69
Y. Trope, N. Liberman, Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 117, 440–463 (2010).
70
S. T. Fiske, Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 67–73 (2018).
71
S. T. Fiske, A. J. C. Cuddy, P. Glick, J. Xu, A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902 (2002).
72
A. Strinić, M. Carlsson, J. Agerström, Occupational stereotypes: Professionals´ warmth and competence perceptions of occupations. Pers. Rev. 51, 603–619 (2021).
73
A. D. I. Kramer, J. E. Guillory, J. T. Hancock, Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 8788–8790 (2014).
74
M. L. Kern et al., The online social self: An open vocabulary approach to personality. Assessment 21, 158–169 (2014).
75
H. C. Shulman, D. M. Markowitz, T. Rogers, Reading dies in complexity: Online news consumers prefer simple writing. Science. Sci. Adv. 10, adn2555 (2024).
76
D. M. Markowitz, Instrumental goal activation increases online petition support across languages. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 124, 1133–1145 (2023).
77
S. C. Matz, J. J. Gladstone, D. Stillwell, In a world of big data, small effects can still matter: A reply to Boyce, Daly, Hounkpatin, and Wood (2017). Psychol. Sci. 28, 547–550 (2017).
78
H. R. Markus, S. Kitayama, Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253 (1991).
79
M. J. Gelfand, L. H. Nishii, J. L. Raver, On the nature and importance of cultural tightness-looseness. J. Appl. Psychol. 91, 1225–1244 (2006).
80
R. Krznaric, The Good Ancestor: A Radical Prescription for Long-Term Thinking (The Experiment, LLC, 2021).
81
G. H. Elder Jr., The life course as developmental theory. Child Dev. 69, 1–12 (1998).
82
83
84
J. W. Pennebaker, R. L. Boyd, R. J. Booth, A. Ashokkumar, M. E. Francis, Linguistic inquiry and word count (LIWC-22) (Version 1.11.0, Pennebaker Conglomerates, 2022). https://liwc.app/. Accessed 12 January 2025.